Saturday, 31 May 2014

Fight, or flight? ... or die.

As a predator approaches, prey make escape decisions based on trade-offs between fitness consequences of fleeing and not fleeing (Cooper & Wilson, 2007). Sometimes, escaping is a high cost when there is an opportunity to stay and increasing their fitness instead. This is especially evident in some males, which are less wary of approaching predators when fleeing means that they are abandoning courtship or mate guarding (Cooper & Wilson, 2007).


For example, male striped plateau lizards (Sceloporus virgatus) permit closer approach before fleeing when given the opportunity to court or perform aggressive behaviour, whereas females flee much sooner as they cannot justify taking the risk of staying (Cooper & Wilson, 2007). 

Female Sceloporus virgatus
Author unknown 
The primary measure of weariness is the flight initiation distance (FID - the distance between predator and prey when escape begins) and the escape theory predicts that FID increases with degree of predation risk of a predator and decreases with cost of escaping (Cooper & Wilson, 2007). This demonstrates a clear trade-off between staying to increase fitness/exposing itself to more danger and fleeing to survive/decreasing the opportunity to increase its fitness.

Another option, which is rather strange… is to essentially commit suicide.



This is an act of altruism which is commonly found in eusocial groups of insect species, especially ants, and it obvisouly decreases the actor’s fitness whilst benefiting the rest of the colony (Shorter & Rueppell, 2012). Whilst other insects display defences such as coloration, toxins, stinging, biting, spraying venom, wing shimmering, and structure building, the  Carpenter Ant (Camponotus saundersi) displays an instantaneous defence known as autothysis, which is basically self-explosion which results in the release of a caustic secretion; killing the attacker (Shorter & Rueppell, 2012).  The ant has an enlarged mandibular gland which contains poisonous chemicals and a glue-like substance, and when threatened they contract their abdominal muscles which such strength that these glands explode and the ant dies. 

Camponotus saundersi committing suicide for the good of the colony
Author: Mark Moffett 

Workers use this type of event either in one-on-one confrontations of territorial defence far away from the nest (Shorter & Rueppell, 2012). In turn, the rest of the colony is protected and reproduce, creating more generations of Kamikaze Ants!

That’s a pretty extreme cost, in my opinion…

References:


Saturday, 24 May 2014

What in the World Am I Looking At!?

Take a look at this animal.

Photo of an adult Saiga
Photographer: Unknown
I assume that most of you are looking at this, asking yourselves; “But...WHY???” I know I was when I first saw it anyway.

This rather interesting looking animal is called a Saiga (Saiga t. tatarica) and it has evolved one of the most extraordinary noses (or proboscis) among the mammalian kingdom. (Frey, et al. 2007) The Saiga is a relatively young species that appear to have originated in central Asia in the late Tertiary (Pliocene) or early Quaternary (Pleistocene). (Frey, et al. 2007) Recent individuals are quite morphologically similar to fossils from Pleistocene Saigas, and so reconstruction of the evolution of this nose is difficult. (Frey, et al. 2007) In fact, the biological role of this nose has not even been fully explained. The most accepted hypothesis is that this contraption initially evolved to filter out dust particles from inhaled air when large herds of these species migrate and kick you copious amounts of dust. (Frey, et al. 2007) The video below shows just how much dust is kicked up during their cross-country migrations.


This type of locomotion (called the ‘amble’) is most energetically advantageous across an open flat semi-arid steppe environment, and it involves the neck being kept horizontal, meaning that the head is almost permanently immersed in a dust cloud. (Frey, et al. 2007) It has been suggested that a selection pressure for the evolution of a dust filter has led to the extraordinary size and shape of these noses in both sexes. (Frey, et al. 2007)

Diagram showing the anatomical structure of the Saiga proboscis. This includes the lateral vestibular recess, alarobasal fold, nasal septum and the vocal tract. The nasal passage is indicated in red and the oral passage is indicated in blue. (Frey, et al. 2007)

There is also a sexual dimorphism evident in nose sizes, as males have a longer proboscis, especially when performing a rut. (Frey, et al. 2007) This display occurs from December to January, where male establish territories and gather (sometimes up to 50) females, defending them in severe fights against rival males. (Frey, et al. 2007)


Diagram showing the nose extension in an adult male saiga during the rut. The image on the top is the resting position and on the bottom is the calling posture. (Frey, et al. 2007)
Females also call, but their noses are often more relaxed during this process, and it is used for a different reason, most likely to call to their young. (Frey, et al. 2007) Another suggested reason for the evolution of this nose is heat loss. The environment these animals live in can be as hot as 50oC, which is enough to cause brain damage in most animals, however the passage of air through the proboscis cools down the inhaled air and in turn cools down the body of this amazing animal. (Frey, et al. 2007)

The following is a hypothetical step-by-step model for the evolution of the Saiga nose, taking into consideration is specialization for vocalizing and the ecological constraints found in dry-steppe environment. (Frey, et al. 2007)
  1. Increasing use of the amble as a mode of fast locomotion by a smaller-sized bovid promoted the initial evolution of the unique nose, with slit-like nostrils
  2. A low head position is advantageous for ambling (better extension of forelimbs through greater freedom of shoulder musculature). However, due to the large dust clouds produced by this mode of transport, the slit-like nostrils have to be opened more widely. This mean means that small nasal vestibulum is more susceptible to getting clogged with dust, causing a reduced efficiency of breathing (tradeoff!)
  3. This is followed by the evolution of slightly larger nasal vestibula, at the expense of the nasal cavity proper in both sexes. These changes lead to a large difference in the dimensions of the nasal cavities compared to a typical bovid nose.
  4. These transformations may have led to a reduced efficiency of counter-current exchange of heat and water vapour effected by the respiratory region, and so the advantages of the derived nose structure must overcompensated those disadvantages (another trade-off!)
  5.  The increasing demand for getting rid of mucus and dust from the nasal vestibulum caused regular forced expirations through the nose, which lead to a preferential calling through the nose rather than through the mouth in both sexes.
  6. Increasing use of the nasal airway by adult males during rutting calls instigated the evolution of sexual dimorphism by mechanisms of sexual selection (a mechanism discussed in my previous blogs)
  7. Nasal, instead of oral, roaring was favoured by selection. Roaring loudly through the nose signals an exaggerated body size to conspecifics when compared to roaring through the mouth (a constraint to the further reduction of the nasal cavity proper and enlargement of the nasal vestibulum means the acoustic signal is an honest indicator of body size which provides reliable information for rivals as well as females).


I realise this post is a bit off topic, considering my blog is about trade-offs and constraints. However, I mentioned in my first post how intrigued I am by the mechanisms that cause species to develop weird and wonderful body parts and behaviours that just don’t seem to make sense at first glance. The Saiga is a perfect example of a creature that, when first seen, evokes feelings of curiosity and wonderment as to how and why something has evolved like this. Upon further investigation, as we can see, there is a very legitimate reason for having such a ridiculously large nose!

This amazing creature is unfortunately critically endangered due to high levels of poaching, and their numbers have declined by 95% in just the past 15 years. (The Saiga Conservation Alliance, 2014) The Saiga Conservation Alliance aims to restore populations and save them from imminent extinction. They welcome support from the public. Their link is: http://www.saiga-conservation.com/ if you are interested in further information.

References:
  • Frey, R., Volodin, I. & Volodina, E. 2007, "A nose that roars: anatomical specializations and behavioural features of rutting male saiga", Journal of Anatomy, vol. 211, no. 6, pp. 717-736.
  • The Saiga Conservation Alliance, 2014, 25th May 2014 <http://www.saiga-conservation.com/>
  • Viewed 23rd May 2014 <http://www.listoid.com/list/18>

Thursday, 8 May 2014

Sneaky cheaters!

Last week I explored the concept of animal communication through honest signalling, in which a signaller sends a signal of quality to a receiver. In order for an individual to be able to create and carry a costly trait, they must have a high fitness. Predators learn that they should try and go for a weaker individual instead, intraspecific competitors learn it would be futile to approach them for fights, and members of the opposite sex are honestly signalled that they would be a mate with a higher fitness.

This concept has been widely researched and discussed in the last few decades, and the accepted idea has gone from organisms displaying implicit honesty, to no honesty and no long-term communication at all, and finally to cost-reinforced honesty (which is the conventional explanation of today) (Rowell et al. 2006).

You see, as always, there are going to be some which manage to break the rules and sneak through the system. If they can do this without creating too much fuss whilst in the meantime increasing their fitness, then why not?

'
Sourced from:http://flickrhivemind.net/User/BrentDPayne/Interesting
Photographer unknown

 These cheeky individuals can cheat by giving a dishonest signal. An example where deception in intraspecific fights is important is when stomatopods of the species Gonodactylus bredini fight over ownership of a cavity (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). This species exhibits a display called the meral spread, which involves leaning out (usually keeping their body in the burrow) to reveal the meral spot, a small, colourful depression on the underside of the appendage, and to make the individual appear large and threatening (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). Normally the size of this display provides accurate information about the aggressiveness of the signaller. This is used when defending homes, and also when females defend their eggs (Adams & Caldwell, 1990).

Peacock mantis shrimp (different species of mantis shrimp) engaging in the meral display
Photo by: Linda Cline

 However, research has shown that newly moulted individuals sometimes also exhibit this display to bluff to their components (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). These weakened individuals are left with a very vulnerable soft exoskeleton that cannot withstand blows, and so as they lack the ability to actually defend themselves in a fight, the display in this case can be interpreted as bluffs (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). The body is not seen and so competitors do not see that the individual is weakened.

Bluffing has also been seen in a species of Green Tree Frogs, Rana clamitans, where some small males exaggerate their quality by lowering their acoustic pitch to resemble that of larger males (Bee et al. 2000).


Male Green Tree Frogs 
Rana clamitans  calling to females
Sourced from: 
http://academics.skidmore.edu/wikis/NorthWoods/index.php/Rana_clamitans_melanota_-_Green_Frog
Photographer:  Unknown

 Dishonest advertisement is also seen in species of Fiddler crabs such as Uca lactea mjoebergi, which have been shown to lie about their fighting ability. Males have an enlarged major claw (which looks very impractical in my opinion) which is used both to attract females and fight rivals (Backwell et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009). Most often, those possessing larger claws win the fights, and so we expect larger claws to be an honest signal of increased fitness and fighting ability (Backwell et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009).


Male fiddler crab, Uca mjoebergi Photographer: Tanya Detto 

When a male crab loses a claw, it occasionally re-generates a new one. This new claw (termed a leptochelous) is about the same size as the original, however much weaker (Backwell et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009). However, what’s interesting is that rival crabs are unable to distinguish between the original and weaker claw and are still intimidated by its size and deterred from combat (Backwell et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009). As the crab with the regenerated claw would be unable to back up his ‘claims’ that he is a strong male if he ever actually got into a fight, this is seen also a prime example of a bluff.


Watch the video below to see two male Fiddler crabs battling it off:



So if escaping battles and predators and getting the ladies with minimal energy and cost is this easy, why doesn’t everyone do it?

Imagine a group of foraging birds, which signal a warning call to each other whenever predators are near. The receiver (predator) receives an honest signal that its prey are aware that it is there, and so gets the message that he/she should move on and try and find a group of unsuspecting birds instead. If cheats gave random false alarms, just in case a predator is nearby, then individuals would soon learn that there is no point in fleeing as it is probably a false alarm and it’s likely that there isn’t anything preying on them, just like the other 43 false alarms of that morning.

Likewise, in the case of the mantis shrimp mentioned above, if mantis shrimp continuously bluffed opponents successfully every time, then all mantis shrimp would evolve to bluff by using the technique of meral spread display, and it would eventually no longer be effective and fall into disuse.

Over time every dishonest signal would weaken the integrity of the whole system (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Rowell et al. 2006). For this reason, in order to establish and maintain what is called an Evolutionary Stable strategy (ESS), the proportion of dishonest signals must be low. That is, bluffing with the display must only work for certain individuals (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Rowell et al. 2006).


In fact, at the ESS the very strongest and the very weakest members of the population threaten while animals of intermediate strength do not (Rowell et al. 2006). Think about the mantis shrimp again... When strong mantis shrimp display, they may deter the conspecific, however even if they don’t they are probably able to win the battle as their display is in fact honest of their superior fighting ability. When a newly molted mantis shrimp bluffs, it takes the risk that the conspecific might attack, and just hopes that it doesn’t actually have to get in a fight. This is disastrous if the bluff doesn’t work. For this reason, for individuals of mediocre strength the risk isn’t worth it and they are better off not using the technique to ward off competitors and investing their energy in something else (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Rowell et al. 2006). Hence we see a system where only the strongest and the weakest mantis shrimp benefit from bluffing and the bluffing technique will continue to ward off opposing conspecifics as long as this low frequency of individuals bluffing is maintained (Holden, 1995).


References:

  • Adams E.S & Mesterton-Gibbons M 1995, "The cost of threat displays and the stability of deceptive communication", Journal of Theoretical Biology, vol. 175, no. 4, pp. 405-405.
  • Adams, E.S. & Caldwell, R.L. 1990, "Deceptive communication in asymmetric fights of the stomatopod crustacean Gonodactylus bredini", Animal Behaviour, vol. 39, no. 4, pp. 706-716
  • Backwell, P.R., Christy, J.H., Telford, S.R., Jennions, M.D. & Passmore, N.I. 2000, "Dishonest signalling in a fiddler crab", Proceedings. Biological sciences / The Royal Society, vol. 267, no. 1444, pp. 719-724.
  • Bee, M.A., Perrill, S.A. & Owen, P.C. 2000, "Male green frogs lower the pitch of acoustic signals in defense of territories: A possible dishonest signal of size?", Behavioral Ecology, vol. 11, no. 2, pp. 169-177.
  • http://academics.skidmore.edu/wikis/NorthWoods/index.php/Rana_clamitans_melanota_-_Green_Frog (3rd May 2014)
  • http://flickrhivemind.net/User/BrentDPayne/Interesting (3rd May 2014)
  • http://www.blueboard.com/mantis/pics/cline_meral.htm (3rd May 2014)
  • http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2008/11/081111203501.htm (3rd May 2014)
  • https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vPIc8s-V8fM (3rd May 2014)
  • Johnstone, R.A. & Grafen, A. 1993, "Dishonesty and the handicap principle", Animal Behaviour, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 759-764.
  • Lailvaux, S.P., Reaney, L.T. & Backwell, P.R.Y. 2009, "Dishonest Signalling of Fighting Ability and Multiple Performance Traits in the Fiddler Crab Uca mjoebergi", Functional Ecology, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 359-366.
  • Rowell, J.T., Associate Editor: Peter D. Taylor, Editor: Michael C. Whitlock, Ellner, S.P. & Reeve, H.K. 2006, "Why Animals Lie: How Dishonesty and Belief Can Coexist in a Signaling System", The American Naturalist, vol. 168, no. 6, pp. E180-E204.

Sunday, 27 April 2014

Liar, liar, pants on fire…
… or telling the truth is probably wiser!


Before you read my post, watch this video of Springbok leaping high into the air when escaping the predation of the fast cheetah. I promise you, you will be entertained.



Now, some of you might be thinking the same thing as I did when I first saw the video. Why on earth would these animals not expend their energy on running away as fast as possible instead of wasting their time and energy on ballerina displays, as impressive as they might be…

 

Well, funnily enough, impression is exactly what these Springbok’s are trying to do. Through this behaviour (called ‘stotting’ displays), they convey a message to their predator that there is no point in chasing them because they are far too fit and strong. The process of signalling quality to a receiver (in this case a predator) is a concept known as the honest signalling theory, or, the handicap principle (Nur & Hasson 1984).


Honest Signalling Theory/Handicap Principle

 

Whether you are exploring the reef, hiking through valleys or strolling through a forest, you are constantly being bombarded with signals, most of them intended for non-human receivers. These could be in the form of calls, colours, smells, just to name a few. Several of these signals are costly to produce and send, and I have discussed some of these in my past blogs.

 

Check out the other examples below:

 

Baby Northern Cardinals begging loudly for food whilst being fed by their parents:

 


 

Performing these loud cries might be useful in signalling to their parents that they are hungry, however it would also undoubtedly attract predators to the nest.

 

Big Bull Elk rubbing the velvet off their huge antlers 

 

 

These horns are impressive, attractive to females and useful in battle, but incredibly energetically expensive to create and carry (some of them weigh up to 18kg!).

 

So why don’t these animals send a more subtle message to the receiver instead of partaking in these elaborate displays? These signals obviously work well in convincing the receiver as they have evolved through natural selection, but how?

 

The Handicap principle proposed by Amotz Zahavi in the 1970’s suggests that if an organism can signal something costly, it means that it has a higher fitness than other individuals as it can afford the signal in the first place. That is, it is strong and healthy enough to deal with the cost and pass the test of survival, when compared to another individual who has an inferior fitness and therefore cannot afford the costly traits. Through this honest signalling organisms are effectively conveying their ‘quality’ to other individuals (Zahavi & Zahavi 1977).

 

This can be to attract females, such as with the peacock or large rack of antlers. Peacock’s with dull feathers that drag on the ground and collect mites signal to females the lower quality and fitness of that male, and she will find someone else with beautiful feathers (making the grooming worth it in the end!). Likewise, small antlers signify to females that the male cannot “afford” to grow bigger ones and carry them and is therefore of lesser quality than the spunky male with the oversized bone/skin/horns projecting from his head (Nur & Hasson 1984, Grafen 1990, Johnstone 1995)

 


Which one would you choose?

 

 

It can also be to deter predators, in the case of Gazelles and Springbok. Jumping high signals to the predator that they are fit and strong, and that they should probably focus more on chasing the one who keeps tangling his hooves and who can’t even jump 2 feet of the ground (how shameful!). The hard to catch prey benefit by distinguishing themselves from less healthy individuals and escape predation, and the predators benefit by understanding this signal and not wasting their time on prey who are more likely to escape. Other prey do not benefit y being distinguished, however the cost of the leaping signal is so high that they cannot pretend to be a harder-to-catch individual, meaning that is an honest signal! (Nur & Hasson 1984, Yachi 1995)

 

In the case of baby birds, they are informing their parents about how hungry they are. These birds might all pretend that they are hungry and all make noise. However, this takes energy and could also possibly attract predators to the nest. This means nestlings end up honestly conveying their hunger levels as the ones who are truly hungry continue making noise (need for food outweighs the risk of predation), whereas the satisfied ones will stay silent due to the cost of predation (Rodríguez-Gironés et al 1996).

 

Signals have to be honest, or the receiver would eventually evolve to ignore them, leading to the signallers evolving not to send them in the first place as they would be completely useless. And in order for the signals to be honest, they must also be costly. Each signaller makes a choice of the size of their handicap they want to produce, taking into consideration both the cost and the benefit (this is often not a conscious decision, but one that is encoded in the genes and acted upon through natural selection). Stronger individuals can assume greater handicaps at lower costs and hence will grow ‘bigger and better’ ornaments, whereas the same handicap would hinder a weak individual. Consequentially, handicap size is a reliable signal of strength to the receiver. 

 

References:

Grafen, A. 1990, "Biological signals as handicaps", Journal of theoretical biology, vol. 144, no. 4, pp. 517-546.

http://www.shutterstock.com/pic-108779864/stock-photo--three-young-rocky-mountain-elk-bulls-with-spike-antlers.html (26th April 2014)

 

http://www.shutterstock.com/pic-108779921/stock-photo-rocky-mountain-elk-in-habitat-with-beautiful-autumn-leaves.html?src=pp-same_artist-108779864-4 (26th April 2014)

 

http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=1tWLDhJ6mjQ (26th April 2014)

 

http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nPWHdJM8bJ8 (26th April 2014)

 

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=2Sl250079Kk (26th April 2014)

 

JOHNSTONE, R.A. 1995, "SEXUAL SELECTION, HONEST ADVERTISEMENT AND THE HANDICAP PRINCIPLE: REVIEWING THE EVIDENCE", Biological Reviews, vol. 70, no. 1, pp. 1-65.

Nur, N. & Hasson, O. 1984, "Phenotypic plasticity and the handicap principle", Journal of Theoretical Biology, vol. 110, no. 2, pp. 275-297.

Rodríguez-Gironés, M.A., Cotton, P.A. & Kacelnik, A. 1996, "The Evolution of Begging: Signaling and Sibling Competition", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 93, no. 25, pp. 14637-14641.

Shigeo Yachi 1995, "How can Honest Signalling Evolve? The Role of Handicap Principle", Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, vol. 262, no. 1365, pp. 283-288.

Zahavi, A. & Zahavi, A. 1977, "The cost of honesty. Further Remarks on the Handicap Principle", Journal of Theoretical Biology, vol. 67, no. 3, pp. 603-605.