Last week I explored the concept of animal
communication through honest signalling, in which a signaller sends a signal of
quality to a receiver. In order for an individual to be able to create and
carry a costly trait, they must have a high fitness. Predators learn that they
should try and go for a weaker individual instead, intraspecific competitors
learn it would be futile to approach them for fights, and members of the
opposite sex are honestly signalled that they would be a mate with a higher
fitness.
This concept has been widely researched and
discussed in the last few decades, and the accepted idea has gone from organisms displaying implicit
honesty, to no honesty and no long-term
communication at all, and finally to cost-reinforced honesty (which is the
conventional explanation of today) (Rowell et
al. 2006).
You see, as always,
there are going to be some which manage to break the rules and sneak through
the system. If they can do this without creating too much fuss whilst in the
meantime increasing their fitness, then why not?
These cheeky individuals can
cheat by giving a dishonest signal. An example where deception in intraspecific
fights is important is when stomatopods of the species Gonodactylusbredini fight over ownership of a cavity (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). This species exhibits a display called the
meral spread, which involves leaning out (usually keeping their body in the
burrow) to reveal the meral spot, a small, colourful depression on the
underside of the appendage, and to make the individual appear large and
threatening (Adams & Caldwell,
1990).
Normally the size of this display provides accurate
information about the aggressiveness of the signaller. This is used
when defending homes, and also when females defend their eggs (Adams & Caldwell, 1990).
Peacock mantis shrimp (different species of mantis shrimp) engaging in the meral display Photo by: Linda Cline
However,
research has shown that newly moulted individuals sometimes also exhibit this display to bluff to
their components (Adams & Caldwell,
1990). These weakened
individuals are left with a very vulnerable soft exoskeleton that cannot
withstand blows, and so as they lack the ability to actually defend themselves
in a fight, the display in this case can be interpreted as bluffs (Adams & Caldwell, 1990). The body is not seen and so competitors do
not see that the individual is weakened.
Bluffing has also
been seen in a species of Green Tree Frogs, Rana clamitans, wheresome small
males exaggerate their quality by lowering their acoustic pitch to resemble
that of larger males (Bee et al. 2000).
Dishonest
advertisement is also seen in species of Fiddler crabs such asUca lactea mjoebergi,
which have been shown to lie about their fighting ability. Males
have an enlarged major claw (which looks very impractical in my opinion) which
is used both to attract females and fight rivals (Backwell et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009).
Most often, those possessing larger claws win the fights, and so we expect
larger claws to be an honest signal of increased fitness and fighting ability (Backwell
et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009).
Male
fiddler crab, Uca mjoebergi Photographer: Tanya Detto
When a male crab loses a claw,
it occasionally re-generates a new one. This new claw (termed a leptochelous) is about the same size as the original, however
much weaker (Backwell et al. 2000,
Lailvaux et al. 2009). However, what’s
interesting is that rival crabs are unable to distinguish between the original
and weaker claw and are still intimidated by its size and deterred from combat (Backwell
et al. 2000, Lailvaux et al. 2009). As the crab with the
regenerated claw would be unable to back up his ‘claims’ that he is a strong
male if he ever actually got into a fight, this is seen also a prime example of
a bluff.
Watch the video below to see
two male Fiddler crabs battling it off:
So
if escaping battles and predators and getting the ladies with minimal energy
and cost is this easy, why doesn’t everyone do it?
Imagine a group of
foraging birds, which signal a warning call to each other whenever predators
are near. The receiver (predator) receives an honest signal that its prey are
aware that it is there, and so gets the message that he/she should move on and
try and find a group of unsuspecting birds instead. If cheats gave random false
alarms, just in case a predator is nearby, then individuals would soon learn
that there is no point in fleeing as it is probably a false alarm and it’s
likely that there isn’t anything preying on them, just like the other 43 false
alarms of that morning.
Likewise, in the case
of the mantis shrimp mentioned above, if mantis shrimp continuously bluffed
opponents successfully every time, then all mantis shrimp would evolve to bluff
by using the technique of meral spread display, and it would eventually no
longer be effective and fall into disuse.
Over time every dishonest signal
would weaken the integrity of the whole system(Adams
& Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Rowell et
al. 2006). For this reason,
in order to establish and maintain what is called an Evolutionary Stable
strategy (ESS), the proportion of dishonest signals must be low. That is, bluffing
with the display must only work for certain individuals (Adams
& Mesterton-Gibbons, 1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Rowell et
al. 2006).
In fact, at the ESS the very strongest and
the very weakest members of the population threaten while animals of
intermediate strength do not(Rowell et al. 2006). Think about the mantis
shrimp again... When strong mantis
shrimp display, they may deter the conspecific, however even if they don’t they
are probably able to win the battle as their display is in fact honest of their
superior fighting ability. When a newly molted mantis shrimp bluffs, it takes
the risk that the conspecific might attack, and just hopes that it doesn’t
actually have to get in a fight. This is disastrous if the bluff doesn’t work.
For this reason, for individuals of mediocre strength the risk isn’t worth it
and they are better off not using the technique to ward off competitors and
investing their energy in something else (Adams & Mesterton-Gibbons,
1995; Johnstone & Grafen, 1993;
Rowell et al. 2006). Hence we see a system where only the strongest and the weakest mantis shrimp benefit from bluffing and
the bluffing technique will continue to ward off opposing conspecifics as long
as this low frequency of individuals bluffing is maintained (Holden, 1995).
References:
Adams E.S &
Mesterton-Gibbons M 1995, "The cost of threat displays and the stability
of deceptive communication", Journal of Theoretical Biology, vol.
175, no. 4, pp. 405-405.
Adams, E.S. & Caldwell,
R.L. 1990, "Deceptive communication in asymmetric fights of the stomatopod
crustacean Gonodactylus bredini", Animal Behaviour, vol. 39, no. 4,
pp. 706-716
Backwell, P.R., Christy,
J.H., Telford, S.R., Jennions, M.D. & Passmore, N.I. 2000, "Dishonest
signalling in a fiddler crab", Proceedings. Biological sciences / The
Royal Society, vol. 267, no. 1444, pp. 719-724.
Bee, M.A.,
Perrill, S.A. & Owen, P.C. 2000, "Male green frogs lower the pitch of
acoustic signals in defense of territories: A possible dishonest signal of
size?",Behavioral
Ecology,vol. 11, no. 2, pp.
169-177.
Johnstone, R.A. &
Grafen, A. 1993, "Dishonesty and the handicap principle", Animal
Behaviour, vol. 46, no. 4, pp. 759-764.
Lailvaux, S.P., Reaney,
L.T. & Backwell, P.R.Y. 2009, "Dishonest Signalling of Fighting
Ability and Multiple Performance Traits in the Fiddler Crab Uca
mjoebergi", Functional Ecology, vol. 23, no. 2, pp. 359-366.
Rowell, J.T., Associate
Editor: Peter D. Taylor, Editor: Michael C. Whitlock, Ellner, S.P. & Reeve,
H.K. 2006, "Why Animals Lie: How Dishonesty and Belief Can Coexist in a
Signaling System", The American Naturalist, vol. 168, no. 6, pp.
E180-E204.
A really great introduction to the world of cheating! Some excellent examples! How do humans fit into this mix? Given our cognitive abilities, we are quite capable of bluffing and cheating, without any real fear of having to engage in energetically costly combats, so why don’t more humans cheat and bluff? Does punishment play a role in maintaining the status quo in these types of systems? Great post!
Cool! "Sneaky Cheaters" was an interesting read :) is there a trade-off involved? Perhaps it maintains genetic variability within the population of these 'weaker' traits that are being concealed. Is 'sneaky mating' (I'm pretty sure it is called something else concerning humans) similar to the "Sneaky Cheaters" concept? (or if you don't answer these questions... does this mean I can cheat in our next noughts and crosses game?! hehe)
While cheating and deceiving the females into thinking the male is of a higher fitness and so potentially lead to mating, it is different to sneaky mating. Sneaky mating is more males using a strategy that allows him to more stealthily access a female partner, while often trying to avoid larger, stronger males.
For example, in the Horned beetles (Onthophagus acuminatus), some males will be larger in size and develop larger horns and others will be smaller and weaker in size (depending on different nutritious conditions during development that affect adult body size). These varying phenotypes will lead individual males to adopt different mating strategies. Those that are larger and stronger guard the entrance to tunnels where the female is resting/feeding and will fight any male that tries to enter. Smaller males have no chance of winning a fight so they develop a sneaky strategy and dig tunnels INTO the female’s tunnel to get to her without the larger mate noticing!
Very sneaky! Interesting read :)
ReplyDeleteA really great introduction to the world of cheating! Some excellent examples! How do humans fit into this mix? Given our cognitive abilities, we are quite capable of bluffing and cheating, without any real fear of having to engage in energetically costly combats, so why don’t more humans cheat and bluff? Does punishment play a role in maintaining the status quo in these types of systems? Great post!
ReplyDeleteCool! "Sneaky Cheaters" was an interesting read :) is there a trade-off involved? Perhaps it maintains genetic variability within the population of these 'weaker' traits that are being concealed.
ReplyDeleteIs 'sneaky mating' (I'm pretty sure it is called something else concerning humans) similar to the "Sneaky Cheaters" concept? (or if you don't answer these questions... does this mean I can cheat in our next noughts and crosses game?! hehe)
While cheating and deceiving the females into thinking the male is of a higher fitness and so potentially lead to mating, it is different to sneaky mating. Sneaky mating is more males using a strategy that allows him to more stealthily access a female partner, while often trying to avoid larger, stronger males.
DeleteFor example, in the Horned beetles (Onthophagus acuminatus), some males will be larger in size and develop larger horns and others will be smaller and weaker in size (depending on different nutritious conditions during development that affect adult body size). These varying phenotypes will lead individual males to adopt different mating strategies. Those that are larger and stronger guard the entrance to tunnels where the female is resting/feeding and will fight any male that tries to enter. Smaller males have no chance of winning a fight so they develop a sneaky strategy and dig tunnels INTO the female’s tunnel to get to her without the larger mate noticing!